The replication of primitive reflexes is
growing in evidence. It has been used effectively in several intervention and
laboratory studies in children and adults. At first glance, this is in conflict
to various therapies that use normal movement such as the Bobath or NDT
approach.
First it is important to understand the
difference between the "replication" and the "reproduction"
of the primitive reflex (PR).
Replication
of the Primitive Reflex
(1) Where appropriate, the sensory stimulus is
slowly and passively replicated.
(2) The movement pattern of the PR is
cognitively and actively replicated. There is no automatic response of the
reflex
Reproduction
of the Primitive Reflex
During either the stimulation of the sensory
reflex or during the replication of the movement pattern, there is an
unconscious reproduction of the primitive reflex. This is non therapeutic and
can be harmful to the motor control pattern. It happens more often with
reflexes that have a tactile sensory stimulus, during the stimulation of the
tactile stimulus (e.g. Babinski, Foot Tendon Guard, Sucking Reflexes). Note: in
neurological patients, the reproduction of the PR is frequently seen during
functional movements or while using normal movement or postural responses to
inhibit the reflex.
These observations likely have contributed to
the misconceptions of PR Replication to inhibit the reflexes. It is critical to
the therapeutic effect that the PR is not reproduced replicated while it is
cognitively being replicated.
The
Neurodevelopmental Process
There is a belief that the neurodevelopmental
process is a sequential development:
Primitive
Reflexes » Sensory Motor » Postural Reflexes » Goal Orientated Movement
Although this is a nice way to explain the
process in textbooks, development doesn't occur as simple as this. Development
uses each of these processes to "feed off" of each other. In general terms it could be said that in
early development the former may be more dominant (primitive reflexes and
sensory motor) and in later development the latter may be more dominant
(postural reflexes and goal orientated movement).
For example, an infant is prone and hears a
sound. This auditory stimulus (sensory)
facilitates an extensor response and the head and trunk move into extension
(primitive reflex). When the head is up (and during) oculomotor is used
(sensory) and the vestibular system and kinesthetic system (sensory) is
stimulated. The infant may then decide to move (goal orientated movement). If it is a roll (postural reflex, but can be
primitive depending on the time frame), there is lower limb, upper limb and
neck "coordination", the vestibular system and kinesthetic systems
are stimulated (sensory) and the tactility system is stimulated (sensory).
In gross terms, as the neurodevelopmental
process and primitive reflexes are going through their normal developmental timeframe,
higher cognitive centers (frontal lobe) are facilitated. The appreciation of
this neurodevelopmental principle has helped us understand and develop a
hypothesis of why the replication of PR helps inhibit the PR.
We
have now observed three cases in infants which have helped develop and support
our hypothesis that it is the cognitive and active replication of the reflex
which inhibits the PR.This in turn facilitates specific higher centers which then inhibit the reflex.
The
observations were:
1.
In
the same setting the infant's reflexes were checked and the infant had a grasp reflex
2.
The
infant made a conscious decision to reach for a toy and grasped it fully and
then brought it back to their body
3.
The
infant's primitive reflex was absent immediately following the activity of
making a cognitive decision to reach for the object and achieve the goal of
grasping it and bringing it back to their body.
In
infants or in adults it may be possible that passively replicating the PR can
also have a clinical benefit. This may be through the mirror neuron system. If
our hypothesis is correct, we need to take advantage of the cognitive aspect of
the process during our clinical application of PR inhibition.
Although
we need further clarification and understanding of the central mechanisms,
these cases and hypothesis provide a plausible hypothesis to pursue.
Sean GT Gibbons BSc (Hons) PT, MSc Ergonomics, PhD (c),
MCPA
References
Edwards
S, Sarwark JF 2005 Infant and Child Motor Development. Clinical Orthopaedics
and Related Research. Number 434: 33–39
Gibbons SGT 2009 Neurological soft signs are
present more often and to a greater extent in adults with chronic low back pain
with cognitive learning deficits. Manual Therapy. 14
(S1): S20
Gibbons SGT 2009 Primitive reflex inhibition
and sensory motor training improves cognitive learning function and symptoms in
chronic disabling low back pain: A case series. Manual Therapy. 14 (S1): S24
Gibbons SGT 2011 Neurocognitive and sensorimotor deficits
represent an important sub-group for whiplash associated disorders. Fifth International
Whiplash Trauma Congress. Aug 24-28; Lund, Sweden. J
Rehabil Med 2011; Suppl 50: 23
Gilfoyle
EM Grady AP Moore JC 1990 Children Adapt. A Theory of Sensoirmotor-Sensory
Development. 2nd Ed. SLACK Incorporated, Thorofare
Goddard
S 2005 Reflexes, Learning and Behavior. Fern Ridge Press. Eugene, Oregon. USA
Jordan-Black
JA 2005 The effects of the Primary Movement programme on the academic
performance of children attending ordinary primary school. Journal of Research
in Special Educational Needs. 5 (3): 101–111
McPhillips
M, et al 2000 Effects of replicating primary-reflex movements on specific
reading difficulties in children: a randomized, double-blind, controlled trial.
The Lancet 355: 537-340
Parfrey K,
Gibbons SGT, Drinkwater EJ, Behm DG 2014 Head and limb position influence
superficial EMG of abdominals during an abdominal hollowing exercise. BMC
Musculoskeletal Disorders. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2474/15/52
No comments:
Post a Comment